home news forum careers events suppliers solutions markets expos directories catalogs resources advertise contacts
 
Forum Page

Forum
Forum sources  
All Africa Asia/Pacific Europe Latin America Middle East North America
  Topics
  Species
 

Radio tags reveal how pesticides are impairing bumblebees' ability to forage - Long-term exposure to a neonicotinoid pesticide harms bees' ability to forage for pollen and may be changing their choices of which flowers to visit


London, United Kingdom
July 9, 2014

In a new study published in the British Ecological Society's journalFunctional Ecology, scientists used Radio Frequency Identification (RFID) tags, which are similar to those used by courier firms to track parcels, to monitor the day-to-day behaviour of bumblebees. They found that prolonged pesticide exposure significantly impaired the bees’ foraging activity. 

Lead researcher Dr Richard Gill from Imperial College London, said: “Bees pollinate many of our crops including most of our fruits, vegetables, nuts and coffee. In fact, one in every three mouthfuls of food we eat depends on bee pollination, so it is particularly worrying to see such a chronic impairment of their foraging behaviour.” 

Co-lead researcher Professor Nigel Raine from the University of Guelph, said: “Bees have to learn many things about their environment, including how to collect pollen. Exposure to this neonicotinoid pesticide may be preventing bees from being able to learn essential skills.” 

The scientists, who carried out the research whilst at the Royal Holloway University of London, monitored 259 bumblebees from 40 colonies over a four-week period and measured the impact of two pesticides, neonicotinoid (imidacloprid) and pyrethroid (lambda cyhalothrin). 

Bumblebee colonies that had been exposed to the neonicotinoid pesticide brought back significantly reduced amounts of pollen in comparison to untreated colonies, suggesting that persistent exposure weakens the foraging performance of bumblebees. 

To compensate for the lack of pollen being brought back by each individual bee, the scientists suggest that neonicotinoid-treated colonies also sent out more foragers. 

When scientists looked at the colour of collected pollen they also found evidence to suggest that exposed foragers preferred different flowers to forage on. 

Results also revealed that pesticides are preventing bees from improving their pollen foraging ability as they age. Foragers in untreated colonies carried larger pollen loads as they got older and more experienced. 

The scientists are urging for environmental policy-makers to consider reforms of the risk assessment guidelines for pesticide use. Dr Gill said: “Pesticide risk assessments should consider how prolonged exposure to low levels of pesticide might affect foraging performance. Our data suggest that these tests should be conducted for extended periods to detect the effects of chronic exposure. Reforms to pesticides risk assessments should also include bumblebees, and solitary bees. Current tests only include honeybees, even though their ecology and behaviour differ substantially from other bees.”

The study suggests that pesticide use could have serious knock-on consequences for the growth and survival of colonies. “If pesticides are affecting the normal behaviour of individual bees, this could have serious knock-on consequences for the growth and survival of colonies. Bumblebees may be much more sensitive to pesticide impacts as their colonies contain a few hundred workers at most, compared to tens of thousands in a honeybee colony”, added Professor Raine.

Previous research has tended to focus on the impacts of short-term pesticide exposure over a period of 48 hours. But as current farming techniques rely heavily on pesticides to produce and maintain high crop yields, it is likely that bees in the field are exposed to pesticides over a much longer period. 

This is the first study to accurately examine long-term exposure and by fitting each bumblebee with a tiny RFID tag, the researchers were able to build up a detailed picture of when individuals went out and returned to their colony throughout the four-week experiment. 

The bees were housed in wooden boxes within a temperature controlled naturally lit laboratory, but were also connected to the natural environment via an outlet hole. The bees were therefore free to forage outside the laboratory at will, and all colonies were provided with artificial nectar (sugar water) in feeders they could choose to visit, hence bees collected all of their pollen and some nectar from real flowers in the field. 

To expose the colonies to pesticides the neonicotinoid was provided in the sugar water and the pyrethroid sprayed onto filter paper which the bees could walk over to reach the feeder. 

The study was funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (BBSRC), the Department for Environment Food & Rural Affairs (Defra), the Natural Environment Research Council(NERC), the Scottish Government and the Wellcome Trust under the Insect Pollinators Initiative, which is managed under the auspices of the Living with Environmental Change (LWEC) partnership.

REFERENCE: Gill RJ, Raine NE . Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposure. Functional Ecology. 9 July 2014. DOI:  10.1111/1365-2435.12292.www.functionalecology.org.

PRESS RELEASE from the British Ecological Society

Long term exposure to a neonicotinoid pesticide damages bees' ability to forage for pollen – and may be changing their choices of which flowers to visit – according to research conducted at Royal Holloway University of London. Based on their findings, risk assessment guidelines for pesticide use should be reformed to give bees more protection, the researchers say.

The study monitored bee activity using Radio Frequency Identification (RFID) tags similar to those used by courier firms to track parcels, and shows long-term pesticide exposure can affect individual bees' day-to-day behaviour. It is published in the British Ecological Society's journal Functional Ecology.

Dr Richard Gill (now at Imperial College London) and Professor Nigel Raine (currently at University of Guelph) measured the impact of two pesticides, a neonicotinoid (imidacloprid) and a pyrethroid (lambda cyhalothrin), either singly or in combination, on the behaviour of individual bumblebees from 40 colonies over a 4-week period.

The bees were free to forage outside the laboratory at will, and all colonies were provided with artificial nectar (sugar water) in feeders they could choose to visit, hence bees collected all of their pollen and some nectar from real flowers in the field. For treated colonies, the neonicotinoid was provided in the sugar water and the pyrethroid sprayed onto filter paper which the bees could walk over to reach the feeder.

By fitting each bee with a tiny RFID tag, the researchers were able to build up a detailed picture of when individuals went out and returned to their colony throughout the 4-week experiment. As well as monitoring the foraging activity of individual RFID-tagged bees, they also recorded how much pollen each bee collected and the colour of the pollen to determine the floral source.

Bees from untreated colonies became better at collecting pollen as they gained foraging experience, while the pollen collection success of bees exposed to the neonicotinoid pesticide declined over time. Neonicotinoid treated colonies sent out more foragers to try and compensate for the lack of pollen being brought back by each individual bee.

According to Professor Raine: “Bees have to learn many things about their environment, including how to collect pollen from flowers. Exposure to this neonicotinoid pesticide seems to prevent bees from being able to learn these essential skills. As well as collecting less pollen, we found that the flower preferences of neonicotinoid exposed bees were different to those of foraging bees from untreated colonies.”

The results suggest that risk assessment guidelines for pesticide use should be reformed. “Pesticide risk assessments should consider how prolonged exposure to low levels of pesticide might affect foraging performance. Our data suggest that these tests should be conducted for extended periods to detect the effects of chronic exposure.

“Reforms to pesticides risk assessments should also include bumblebees, and solitary bees. Current tests only include honeybees, even though their ecology and behaviour differ substantially from other bees,” Gill explained.

“If pesticides are affecting the normal behaviour of individual bees, this could have serious knock-on consequences for the growth and survival of colonies. Bumblebees may be much more sensitive to pesticide impacts as their colonies contain a few hundred workers at most, compared to tens of thousands in a honeybee colony,” Raine said.

The study was funded by the Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council, Defra, the Natural Environment Research Council (NERC), the Scottish Government and the Wellcome Trust under the Insect Pollinators Initiative, which is managed under the auspices of the Living with Environmental Change (LWEC) partnership.

Gill RJ, Raine NE (2014). Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposure. Functional Ecology doi: 10.1111/1365-2435.12292, is published on 9 July 2014 at www.functionalecology.org.
 



More news from:
    . Imperial College London
    . British Ecological Society


Website: http://www3.imperial.ac.uk

Published: July 9, 2014



SeedQuest does not necessarily endorse the factual analyses and opinions
presented on this Forum, nor can it verify their validity.

 

 

12 books on plant breeding, classic, modern and fun
 

12 livres sur l'amélioration des plantes : classiques, modernes et amusants

 
 

The Triumph of Seeds

How Grains, Nuts, Kernels, Pulses, and Pips Conquered the Plant Kingdom and Shaped Human History

By Thor Hanson 

Basic Books

 
 

 

 

Hybrid
The History and Science of Plant Breeding
 

Noel Kingsbury
The University of Chicago Press

 

 
1997-2009 archive
of the FORUM section
.

 


Copyright @ 1992-2024 SeedQuest - All rights reserved